A Constructivist Pedagogy

 

 

Constructivism as a theory of knowing can be summarized in the following principles:

·         Knowledge consists of past constructions-- the world is known through a framework that transforms, organizes, and interprets one's perceptions

·         Constructions develop through the processes of assimilation and accommodation, by which old concepts are adapted and altered to fit a logical framework

·         Learning is more an organic process of invention than a process of accumulation—learning involves different frameworks of understanding which one produces in structural leaps in cognition made throughout development

·         Meaningful learning occurs through reflection and resolution of cognitive conflict, and this process brings coherence to earlier fragmented and incomplete understanding.

Most of the new national science programs are now being influenced to various degrees by an emerging understanding of the way children learn science. The view now is that success in learning requires the student's engagement and effort. It also requires his or her attention to central ideas, the "conceptual themes" of science. Learning is seen to be a complex phenomenon involving various learning styles.

Instead of being passive recipients of data from the outside, the child's memory and meaning are constructions, concepts that are influenced by prior knowledge and other factors. The child using beliefs, concepts, and frameworks, which were created earlier, processes incoming stimuli. As the child solves problems, interacts with others, and adapts to circumstances in life, concepts-- models of the world and how it works-- are added and developed.

A child's prior knowledge about a natural phenomenon influences his or her science learning as Michael Watts (1991) puts it,

"We come to understand things in terms of what we already understand; if we cannot lock new ideas into the ideas we have already generated, then new experiences become somewhat meaningless" .

In learning science, prior knowledge has been demonstrated to be powerfully facilitative or, the opposite, obstructive. The science education literature is full of studies of children's naive science, their alternative conceptions (not misconceptions, because no conception can be proven to replicate physical reality). From this work we can say that some of a child's prior conceptions are common among children of particular developmental levels, while others are idiosyncratic. Naive conceptions often are robust and resistant to change through construction, others are amenable to change.

For the child, as well as for us all, coming to understand something is a creative act. Active participation is a must. The various conceptual worlds represent, or model the physical world (the ‘real’ world and are always interpretations of some text, a written text or a text of nature. The constructions we make help us interpret experience. Our models, like those of scientists, change in response to everyday 'reality testing' and may be replaced by those that work better.

Not all concepts function at the same level. Our conceptual worlds are organized in an hierarchy: some concepts overarch others, some ideas provide a framework for others. Particular ideas are key to understanding others. For example, Newton's concept of force is critical to understanding his mechanics. As the child constructs his or her conceptual world, initial understandings often may be fragmentary and incoherent. Conceptual coherence can develop in big jumps, as insight. A conceptual world can become completely reorganized after a particular insight or upon the resolution of a conflict. The key processes that lead to concept development are reflection and problem solving. Cognitive change includes aspects of restructuring, invention, and directionality. The child may appropriate the adult meaning.

                    Matrix